, 2000) and the CoS In addition, voxels

that were signif

, 2000) and the CoS. In addition, voxels

that were significantly more active during the scrambled objects condition were Selleck Navitoclax delineated in and around the calcarine fissure as an early visual ROI (EarlyVis; Figure S2). We were able to delineate the LO and EarlyVis ROIs in all 14 participants, the pFs ROI in 12 participants, the left CoS in 10 participants, and the right CoS in 8 participants. Second, another set of ROIs was generated from the data obtained in the camouflage run itself by contrasting activity during the SOL stage of all three event types (SPONT, REM, and NotREM) with activity during the time period of baseline (blank) trials and thresholding at q < 0.05. Note that by collapsing across all event types, the resulting statistical map, this website and the ROIs extracted from it, was unbiased with respect to subsequent memory performance (Kensinger and Corkin, 2004). This contrast resulted in extensive activations in visual as well as frontal areas, and also in prominent activation clusters in bilateral amygdala (for the full list of activations, see Table S1). To examine more closely the activity in those brain regions that were particularly engaged during SOL (and for which we did not have independent localizer data), we delineated from the results of this contrast ROIs in the frontal cortex (in the lateral orbital gyrus and in the inferior frontal sulcus) and in the amygdala (as identified by anatomical

markers; Duvernoy, 1999). Following Johnstone et al. (2005), the amygdala ROI was defined separately for each subject. For each participant we took the clusters of three contiguous functional voxels activated in conjunction Rebamipide with group level activation. We were able to delineate activation in the left amygdala for 11 participants, but in the right amygdala, only for 6. (Even when the threshold

for the right amygdala was relaxed to q < 0.15, we were able to delineate the right amygdala ROI for only eight subjects, and the additional data did not change the results.) Finally, we delineated the hippocampus based on the high-resolution T1-weighted MRI images and on established anatomical landmarks (Pruessner et al., 2000). Three separate hippocampal ROIs were defined for each observer separately: head, body, and tail. For each of the ROIs we extracted the time course obtained during the camouflage Study session, separately in each participant. The time courses were linearly interpolated from the TR resolution (2 s) to 1 s resolution, to fit the protocol time course, and transformed into percent signal change, based on the two TRs preceding each event. (This and all other time course calculations were done using Matlab, The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, version 6.1, 2001.) ROI time course data were first analyzed by computing event-triggered averages for each event type (SPONT, REM, and NotREM) separately for each stage in the trial (CAM1, SOL, CAM2; Figure 3A).

Without attention, the competition between features appears to be

Without attention, the competition between features appears to be suspended, with binocular neurons being driven by features from both eyes. A total of 17 observers (13 naive to the purposes of the experiment) participated in experiment

1. Four naive observers participated in experiment 2. Four observers (two naive) participated in experiment 3. All subjects had normal or corrected to normal vision. In experiment 1, one subject was tested twice, and four subjects were eliminated from analysis due to low SNR, poor rivalry quality, or failure to follow task instructions. Thirteen subjects’ data (nine naive) were included for analysis. The experimental protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Minnesota. Each trial lasted 30 s, and data were collected from 12 trials in each condition.

In the attended conditions, subjects reported their perception Trichostatin A concentration by pressing one of two buttons corresponding to the dominance of the red or the green BMS-354825 purchase checkerboard. In the unattended conditions, subjects ignored the checkerboards and performed a demanding color-shape conjunction detection task on the central fixation point. Left and right eye stimuli were dichoptically presented using a mirror stereoscope for all three experiments. A chinrest was used to minimize subjects’ head movement. For experiments 1 and 2, EEG data were recorded using a 64-channel Neuroscan SynAmps RT system (Compumedics of Neuroscan) with a band-pass filter from DC to 200 Hz, and digitized at 1000 Hz. A 64-channel Ag-AgCl electrode cap was used, but only six posterior channels were used for analysis (for some subjects, we only collected data from these six electrodes), including Oz, POz, O1, O2, CB1, and CB2. A surface Laplacian spatial filter was applied on the continuous EEG data to minimize common noise (Hjorth, 1975); signals from the five electrodes surrounding Oz were averaged then subtracted from the signal from Oz. The resultant was band-pass filtered from 1 to 30 Hz. An adaptive RLS filter (Tang and Norcia, 1995) was used to extract the amplitude of the tagged frequencies over

time. The rivalry index was computed by dividing the peak amplitude of the counterphase signal by the amplitude of the aligned signal. This was done separately for alignment at peaks and troughs, and the results were averaged. MATLAB (MathWorks) was used for power spectrum analysis. In experiment 3, a 128-channel electrode cap and two 64-channel SynAmps RT amplifiers were used, with the same recording parameters as experiments 1 and 2. Source localization was performed using CURRY 6 (Compumedics Neuroscan) and custom MATLAB scripts. Structural MR images were collected on a 3T Siemens Trio scanner (T1 MPRAGE, 1 mm isotropic voxels), and three-layer boundary element models (Hämäläinen and Sarvas, 1989 and He et al.

While there are good reasons to believe that DA modulates transmi

While there are good reasons to believe that DA modulates transmitter release by directly activating presynaptic DA receptors, Raf inhibitor experimental evidence formally excluding the involvement of postsynaptic receptors is rare, especially at synapses in which DA receptors are expressed both pre- and postsynaptically. Using paired recordings from synaptically connected SPNs, Tecuapetla et al. (2009) showed that DA acting on D2 but not D1 receptors depresses GABA release from iSPNs (expressing D2 receptors)

onto dSPNs (expressing D1 receptors), providing compelling evidence for a direct presynaptic locus of action. In striatum, activation of D2 receptors diminishes BTK signaling pathway inhibitor presynaptic release of glutamate from corticostriatal afferents (Bamford et al., 2004; Higley and Sabatini, 2010; Salgado et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2012). Although commonly attributed to activation of presynaptic

D2 receptors, DA and D2 receptor agonists have small (Wang et al., 2012) or negligible effects on mEPSCs (André et al., 2010; Nicola and Malenka, 1998), the reduction in evoked glutamate release scales with afferent stimulation frequency (Bamford et al., 2004; Yin and Lovinger, 2006) and is prevented by postsynaptic Ca2+ buffering as well as pharmacological and genetic blockade of metabotropic glutamate and endocannabinoid receptors (Tozzi et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2012; Yin and Lovinger, 2006). While these studies do not exclude a role for presynaptic D2 receptors, Montelukast Sodium they suggest that under conditions of elevated

synaptic activity, DA and glutamate interact postsynaptically to decrease synaptic drive through the synthesis of endocannabinoid retrograde messengers. A similar inhibitory feedback pathway relying on postsynaptic release of adenosine has been proposed downstream of D1-like and NMDA receptors in ventral striatum (Harvey and Lacey, 1997; Wang et al., 2012), though this has not been universally observed (Nicola and Malenka, 1997). Using optical imaging of exocytic events and electrophysiological recordings from EGFP-labeled dSPNs and iSPNs in BAC transgenic mice, Wang et al. (2012) recently dissected the presumed pre- and postsynaptic effects of D1 and D2 receptors on glutamate release from corticoaccumbal afferents. Under conditions of minimal synaptic activity (i.e., in TTX), their studies revealed slight presynaptic excitatory and inhibitory effects of D1- and D2-like receptor agonists on glutamate release, respectively. Under conditions of moderate to high corticoaccumbal activity (spontaneous and evoked EPSCs), stimulation of D1- and D2-like receptors both evoked a more pronounced decrease in glutamate release that originated postsynaptically and occluded presynaptic contributions.

For each neuron, we computed a STRF based on the spiking response

For each neuron, we computed a STRF based on the spiking responses to all but one of 15 songs, and we validated each STRF by using it to predict the response to the song not used during STRF estimation. The STRFs of midbrain, primary AC, and higher-level AC NS neurons showed clear tuning for particular acoustic features (Figure S6D) and could be used to accurately MS-275 in vivo predict neural responses to novel stimuli (Figure S6E). In

contrast, the acoustic features to which BS neurons in the higher-level AC were sensitive were poorly characterized by STRFs, and STRFs of BS neurons were poor predictors of neural responses to novel stimuli. These results suggest that the responses of BS neurons

may be modulated by more than the short time-scale acoustic features that are typically coded by upstream populations. To determine whether BS neurons were sensitive to long time-scale acoustic information (tens to hundreds of milliseconds), we presented individual notes independent click here of their acoustic context in songs. We reasoned that if BS neurons are highly selective feature detectors that were only sensitive to short time-scale information, they should respond to the same subset of notes when presented independently or in the context of a song. We further predicted that BS neurons should retain their selectivity for some iterations old of a repeated note but not for others. Contrary to these predictions, BS neurons responded to eight times more notes when they were presented independently (in the absence of acoustic context) than in the context of the song (p < 0.05, Wilcoxon; Figures 6A and 6B). Futhermore, when notes were presented independently, BS neurons tended to respond to more iterations of a repeated note than when they were presented in the context of song (see Figure 6A). The finding that BS neurons can respond to notes that do not drive a response during song indicates that preceding notes within a song

suppress a neuron’s response to subsequent notes. To measure the time course of contextual suppression during the playback of song, we systematically increased or decreased the interval between notes that evoked responses and the notes immediately preceding them (Figure 6C). We found that acoustic context influenced BS neuron responses to subsequent notes with interactions lasting at least 100 ms (Figure 6D). The suppression induced by preceding notes did not require that the neuron respond to the preceding notes (e.g., Figure 6C), suggesting that contextual suppression is synaptic rather than due to intrinsic hyperpolarizing currents, which are typically activated after spiking (Cordoba-Rodriguez et al., 1999). Removing the acoustic context had no effect on the number of notes to which NS or primary AC neurons responded (data not shown).

OSN activity was modeled by IOSN=(0 013×sin(0 6πt750)+0 0050

OSN activity was modeled by IOSN=(0.013×sin(0.6πt750)+0.0050XAV-939 in vitro nA, with t in units of 0.1 ms, giving rise to a cycle length of 300 ms. The firing rate models were generated on a multicore processor system with the x86-64 instruction set. The Bogacki-Shampine method was used in MATLAB to solve dRj=−R+f(R,C,Nj)/tjdRj=−R+f(R,C,Nj)/tj, where R   is the firing rate vector, C   the connectivity

matrix, N  j the single neuron parameters, and t  j the membrane time constant for the j  th neuron. The nonlinearity function f   was given by: f(R,C,Nj)=1/(1+e(slopej×(halfj−∑cij×Rj)−Iextj))f(R,C,Nj)=1/(1+e(slopej×(halfj−∑cij×Rj)−Iextj)), whose shape depended on the single cell parameters, t, slope, half, and Iext give in Table S1. The models were assessed for consistency with experimental observations during control as well as the GABAA-clamp conditions. For the first round of selection, models were deemed consistent with the phase difference

between MCs and TCs if the circular cross correlation (Fisher, 1995) between MC and TC firing rate vectors showed a sufficient global maximum (>0.7) within the 180° ± 35° interval. Of these models, those where MC and TC firing rates were neither zero nor saturated were deemed “consistent with control conditions” (total of 1.5 × 104, SB431542 purchase corresponding to 0.03% of all models). This Carnitine palmitoyltransferase II was assessed using the position of the Jacobian in firing rate space. In the second round of selection, models were deemed “consistent with GABAA-clamp results” if MC phase collapsed onto TC phase ± 40° in simulated GABAA-clamp. This resulted in 1,826 models consistent with GABAA-clamp results. To assess the robustness of each of these models, we varied all connectivity parameters simultaneously by different degrees; the maximum variation ranged from 0% to 30% of total synaptic strength (in steps of 10%), where each variation was drawn from a uniform distribution. Each model was varied 20 times

for each jitter range so that a fraction of connectivity still consistent with the GABAA-clamp results could be determined. A sigmoidal fit was used to determine the robustness of each model, defined as the jitter range at which half of the modified connectivity still remained consistent with the experimental results. This robustness varied widely between models (5.02 – 26.68, 9.43 ± 2.78 [mean ± SD] as determined by the sigmoidal fit over the 10%, 20%, 30% jitter values). Nevertheless, the key connectivity features (strong OSN →TC, weak OSN →MC) were maintained. The connectivity matrix closest to the median of all models consistent with GABAA-clamp was implemented in NEURON (Hines and Carnevale, 1997) using published single cell parameters (Cleland and Sethupathy, 2006). The TC parameters were modified from those of the MC by reducing dendritic membrane area (Figure 2I).

Intrigued

by axon guidance and the multitude of signals t

Intrigued

by axon guidance and the multitude of signals that must occur during this complicated biological process, Doxorubicin mw Tony’s laboratory probed the question with a series of biochemical, cellular, and genetic tests in mice focusing on the Eph family of receptor tyrosine kinases and their membrane-bound Ephrin ligands. Their findings changed the way we think about developmental neurobiology. They showed that Ephs, when stimulated by Ephrins, not only lead to signal transduction in the “receptor”-expressing cell but also activate signaling into the “ligand”-expressing cells—and this bidirectional signaling or cellular crosstalk is critical during the intricate process of guiding axons to their correct destinations (Henkemeyer

et al., 1996). The idea that bidirectional cell-cell—or more precisely axon-cell—contact-mediated Eph-Ephrin interactions help instruct the wiring of the brain set the stage for our understanding how this large family of interacting receptors selleck compound library and ligands controls all sorts of cellular migration/adhesion-type events, including neuronal migration, synapse formation, and synaptic plasticity in the brain; the regulation of blood vessel growth throughout the body; midline development of the embryo; and of course stem cell biology. Based on work from Tony’s laboratory and others, new classes of drug discovery were enabled that ultimately led to the development of cell signaling modulators that treat disease—such as Gleevac, Nexavar, Metalloexopeptidase and Zelboraf. And while the SH2 domain and its biological function was his central discovery, he often ventured far away from his comfort zone and was able to tackle questions using myriad tools and model organisms he could get his hands on—from yeast to C. elegans, Drosophila, and Mus musculus—anything he could use to fulfill his desire to understand how cells communicate. His thirst for knowledge was never quenched, and we who had the chance to work in his laboratory got to see firsthand how excited and passionate his never-ending

love of discovery was. Those who heard him on the lecture circuit and had the chance to interact with him during his travels got just a taste of his brilliance. He truly was an amazing person, with a gentle yet forceful ability to stimulate the minds of the many scientists he had trained and inspired. To quote three enduring words we and others surely remember coming from Tony’s wonderful English accent when discussing an exciting new result or designing a cutting-edge experiment to answer an intriguing question, “I love it. Rest in peace Tony. “
“α-synuclein was independently discovered on multiple occasions, providing important but still incompletely understood clues to its normal function as well as its role in disease.

996, paired t test, n = 5; Figures 5A1–5C) These effects were ob

996, paired t test, n = 5; Figures 5A1–5C). These effects were observed across the entire light intensity input-output relation (Figures

5B1 and 5B2; CCK-Cre: p < 0.05; PV-Cre: p = 0.995; two-way ANOVA with Sidak multiple comparison correction). Thus, ITDP causes a significant iLTD of the CCK IN-mediated inhibitory response in CA1 PNs with little effect on inhibition mediated by PV INs ( Figure 5C, p < 0.0005, unpaired t test, CCK versus PV INs). Our finding that ITDP may involve a selective decrease in CCK IN-mediated inhibition implies that the CCK INs must be major contributors to SC-evoked FFI under basal conditions given the near complete loss of FFI during ITDP. This is somewhat surprising as previous studies using paired recordings between single INs and CA1 PNs indicate that CCK INs are less suited Crizotinib ic50 ABT-737 molecular weight than PV INs for mediating rapid FFI (Daw et al., 2009, Glickfeld and Scanziani, 2006 and Hefft and Jonas, 2005). Because the ChR2-evoked inhibitory response may differ

from that evoked synaptically during FFI, we used pharmacogenetic silencing of CCK INs to determine their contribution to FFI driven by electrical stimulation of the SC inputs. In this pharmacogenetic approach, a Cre-dependent viral vector was used to coexpress a chimeric ligand-gated Cl− channel, the glycine receptor-based pharmacologically selective actuator module (PSAMY115F, L141F-GlyR, referred to as PSAM) with ChR2 (rAAV-CAG-FLEX-ChR2-2A-PSAM; Magnus et al., 2011) in the CA1 region of CCK-ires-Cre mice ( Figures 6A and 6B). Rapid and selective silencing of the virally infected CCK+ neurons was achieved by applying a cognate synthetic ligand (PSEM, pharmacologically selective effector module) that binds to PSAM and activates a shunting Cl− conductance in the PSAM+ neurons ( Magnus et al., 2011). Photostimulation of ChR2 produced Levetiracetam large, CCK IN-mediated IPSCs in uninfected CA1 PNs (Vm +10 mV) that were fully blocked within 6–10 min of bath application of 3 μM PSEM308 ( Lovett-Barron et al., 2012), indicating the efficacy of this approach ( Figure 6C). Silencing of CCK INs by PSEM produced a profound 70% reduction in the IPSC amplitude in CA1 PN soma in

response to electrical stimulation of the SC inputs (from 0.84 ± 0.11 nA to 0.26 ± 0.05 nA, p < 0.001, paired t test, n = 6; Figure 6D1). The CCK INs accounted for the majority of the IPSC evoked by SC stimulation over a range of stimulus intensities (p < 0.0001, SC IPSC, two-way ANOVA with Sidak correction for multiple comparisons; Figure 6D2). Pharmacogenetic removal of CCK INs increased the SC PSP amplitude at the CA1 PN soma by ∼100%, from 4.32 ± 0.35 mV to 8.74 ± 0.92 mV, using a fixed stimulus intensity (50% of spike threshold intensity; p < 0.005, paired t test, n = 6; Figure 6E1). A similar increase was seen over the entire stimulus input-output relation (p < 0.0001, two-way ANOVA with Sidak correction for multiple comparisons, n = 5; Figure 6E2).

Running is the predominant activity, and explosive efforts during

Running is the predominant activity, and explosive efforts during sprints, duels, jumps, and changes of direction are important performance factors, requiring maximal strength and anaerobic power of the neuromuscular system.1, 2, 3 and 4 The physiological and technical demands of the sport

lead coaches and clinicians to continually look for the best methods Selleckchem ABT263 of preparation for the athletes to perform at their optimum. The completion of an active warm-up before training or physical competition has typically been shown to have a positive impact on athletic performance with improvement in power, speed, and agility.5, 6 and 7 Contemporary research has identified the importance of a dynamic warm-up on improving reactive strength and jumping ability in soccer.7 An effective warm-up, however, should not just be seen as essential to performance but also as a mechanism to reduce incidence of injury amongst players. Compliance with specific dynamic warm-up protocols, such as the FIFA 11+, learn more have been shown to decrease injury risk amongst youth soccer players.8 However, the FIFA 11+ warm-up, although well established as a means of reducing injuries, has been reported as not having an effect on performance outcomes in soccer players.1, 9 and 10 Vescovi and VanHeest11 suggested that developing warm-up protocols with not only injury prevention benefits but also performance

benefits, would make it easier to convince coaches to implement such programmes. Some researchers have discussed additions to the FIFA 11+ warm-up protocol to help realise performance enhancements.10 Impellizzeri et al.,10 however, pointed out that any such additions to the FIFA 11+ need to consider fatigue (worsening of performance) and time efficiency to the soccer player. Although the FIFA 11+ has been traditionally investigated over a longer period of time, Zois et al.12 has encouraged researchers to challenge traditional warm-up routines in soccer and how they subsequently effect acute physical qualities of the players. Soligard et al.8 raised some important issues when it comes

to successful warm-up programme to improve performance and reduce injury risk; what is crucial is compliance from the athlete and the coach. Time constraints are seen as a perceived barrier for many coaches to the implementation below of a specific warm-up protocol and the perceived increase in workload.8 As such whole body vibration (WBV) exercise is an acute application that can easily be administered and has been previously identified as an ideal dressing-room based intervention in soccer. It has also been identified as a possible counter to any cool down period between pitch based warm-up and performance, or as a useful addition during tactical discussions.13 Recent investigation has identified acute WBV as a viable method of improving speed in soccer.14 and 15 Turner et al.

01% Evans blue Positive and negative control sera were included

01% Evans blue. Positive and negative control sera were included in each slide and were obtained from animals with positive and negative selleck chemicals serology, respectively, as previously determined by two serological tests (IFAT and ELISA) for both parasites. Slides were examined under fluorescence microscopy and only a bright, linear peripheral fluorescence of the tachyzoites was considered positive. The reciprocal of the highest dilution of serum that gave a positive reaction was considered the end-point titer. Indirect ELISAs were carried out to detect IgG antibodies to T. gondii (ELISA-Tg) and N. caninum (ELISA-Nc), according to Silva

et al. (2007), with modifications. Optimization of the reaction was established in preliminary experiments through block titration of the reagents, by testing simultaneously different antigen concentrations, positive and negative control serum dilutions, biotinylated secondary antibody and enzymatic conjugate dilutions. Microtiter plates were coated with T. gondii or N. caninum (10 μg/mL) soluble antigens and then blocked with PBS containing 0.05% Tween 20 (PBS-T) and skim milk (PBS-TM) at 5%. Next, plates were incubated with sheep sera

diluted 1:64 (ELISA-Tg) or 1:50 (ELISA-Nc) in PBS-TM at 5%. Subsequently, biotinylated rabbit anti-sheep IgG (prepared as described by Lisanti et al., 1988) diluted 1:250 in PBS-TM at 5% GDC-0449 ic50 was added followed by incubation with peroxidase-streptavidin Sitaxentan (Sigma Chemical Co., St. Louis, USA) diluted 1:1000 in PBS-TM at 1%. The reaction was developed by adding the enzyme substrate (0.03% H2O2 and 0.01 M 2,2′-azino-bis-3-ethyl-benzthiazoline sulfonic acid) (ABTS; Sigma) and the optical density (OD) was read at 405 nm in plate reader. Two positive and three negative control sera were included in each plate and were

obtained from animals with positive and negative serology respectively, determined by the two serological tests (IFAT and ELISA) for both parasites. The reaction cut off was calculated as the mean OD for negative control sera plus three standard deviations. Antibody titers were expressed as ELISA index (EI) as described by Silva et al. (2002a) following the formula: EI = OD sample/OD cut off. Values of EI ≥ 1.3 were considered to be positive in order to exclude borderline reactivity values close to EI = 1.0. Immunoblot (IB) was performed to verify the reactivity to T. gondii (IB-Tg) and N. caninum (IB-Nc) immunodominant antigens in all serum samples with discordant results by ELISA and IFAT, as previously described ( Silva et al., 2007). Briefly, T. gondii and N. caninum tachyzoites (108 organisms/mL) were lysed in 4% sodium dodecyl sulfate (SDS) sample buffer and then submitted to electrophoresis in a 12% polyacrylamide gel (SDS-PAGE) under non-reducing conditions ( Laemmli, 1970). Proteins were electrotransferred to nitrocellulose membranes ( Towbin et al.


“Muscular strength can be determined by two components: mu


“Muscular strength can be determined by two components: muscle activation and muscle size. The first of these two components, muscle activation, is the result of efferent output from the central nervous system (CNS).1 This includes the control

of motor unit recruitment (the number of active motor units) and motor unit firing rate (the rate at which they fire). Motor unit recruitment and firing rate are reflected in the amplitude of the interference pattern of the summated PI3K inhibitor action potentials recorded by surface electromyography (sEMG).2 The second component of strength is based on the amount of contractile proteins within skeletal muscle.3, 4 and 5 The amount of contractile tissue can be measured by cross-sectional area (CSA) and anthropometric measures used to infer muscle size.4 and 6 It is widely known that CSA is at least moderately correlated (r = 0.5–0.7) with voluntary strength regardless of gender, age and training status. 5 and 7 The see more relationship between muscle size and force is of sufficient magnitude that the “specific tension” of a muscle is commonly used in musculoskeletal modeling studies to predict force. 8 The specific tension of a muscle is the

force normalized with respect to its CSA. Kroll and colleagues9 extended the research in this field by developing strength prediction equations using non-invasive, simple measures of body weight (BW), body volume, segmental limb lengths much and volumes of the upper limb for both males and females. Multiple regression analysis revealed that the best predictor of elbow flexion strength was BW for males (R = 0.69), and total upper limb volume for females (R = 0.72). Kroll and colleagues 9 also determined that limb girths and lengths predict elbow flexion strength as well as, or better than, segmental limb volumes thereby simplifying the methodology in this area. Given the relationship between muscle activation (sEMG) and force10 and 11 it would seem logical to add this variable to a multiple regression equation that predicts force. An equation that incorporates both anthropometric data and sEMG measurement should

theoretically capture the two components of muscle strength (size and muscle activation) and decrease the standard error of estimate. The present study will therefore determine the relative contributions of body size and muscle activation in a strength prediction equation. The hypothesis of this study is that adding muscle activation (sEMG) to anthropometrics will improve the strength prediction equation. Ninety-six (46 males and 50 females), right-handed college age participants took part in the present study. Each subject was verbally acquainted with the experimental design and provided written, informed consent (REB #02-284). Since this paper attempted to extend the work of Kroll and colleagues9 by adding muscle activation (sEMG), we collected the same anthropometric measurements used in that paper.